Psychology Research Institute / PsyRes
In addition, researchers are required to adhere to the following rules:
For all students and staff members, before a study is implemented, permission of the Ethical Committee is required. The application includes a description of the design, participants, procedures, and methods. Participants are asked to complete a so-called informed consent form and have the right to stop their participation in the experiment at any point. Meanwhile, there exists a behavioural code in the LAB that explicitly outlines the obligations of the participant. At the end of the experiment participants are informed about the research goals (debriefing). Data and informed consent forms need to be stored at least 10 years after data collection is completed as specified in the Data Storage Protocol. Permission of the participant is required for researchers to provide access to data or confidential information to third parties. Personal information that could be linked to the participant should not at any point be related to the identity of the participant. All research follows the European General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR). Please consult the FAQ GDPR and research
Researchers who conduct research in Building D (LAB) are required to follow the regulations of the LAB with regard to getting participants, registration, and payment. This also applies to researchers who conduct online research using the LAB subject pool.
Research data are the core of academic research and should therefore be managed carefully throughout their lifecycle, to prevent data loss and promote data reuse. At the same time, researchers at Psychology often work with sensitive personal data, whose storage and publication is somewhat restricted by the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR). The RDM website of the research institute Psychology, compiled by the data stewards, provides guidelines in the various stages of the research process.
Authorship should without exception be based upon:
a. Substantial contributions to ideas and development thereof, or development and analysis of theoretic models, or data collection, or analysis and interpretation of data. We strongly advise to include a footnote or other text form that specifies the contributions.
b. Preparation of the actual manuscript or critical revision of the article’s intellectual content.
c. Responsibility for the article version that shall be published.
4.1. The above three criteria (4 a, b and c) must all be fulfilled in order to qualify as co-authorship. An administrative relationship, acquisition of funding, collection of data, or general supervision of a research group alone does not constitute authorship.
4.2. Together with the supervisory team PhD candidates should explicitly discuss co-authorship with all possible parties in an early stage. Where the work is directly a result of the PhD project, the PhD will be first author. In other projects, order of authorship should be decided on the basis of importance of contribution and otherwise alphabetically. All agreements should be documented in the PhD Trajectory Plan.
4.3. These criteria should be met for each article separately.
Researchers have to comply with the ‘Netherlands Code of Conduct for Research Integrity 2018’ (Plagiarism), and the ‘Advisory memorandum correct citation practice of the KNAW’ (Selfcitation).
As stated in the Netherlands Code of Conduct for Scientific Practice by the VSNU, scientific activities should be performed scrupulously, but scrupulousness also applies to relations among scientific practitioners and with students. In particular, the education of young scientists requires binding standards for mentorship and supervision. While the main responsibility of supervisors is guiding the academic progress of their trainees, supervisors should assist their trainees in becoming independent researchers and take responsibility for the future scientific generation by ensuring the quality of research and teaching in the future. Researchers should follow the recommendations specified in the UvA Psychology Mentorship and Responsobility of Future Scientific Generations document.
In science, there are ample opportunities for potential conflicts of interests to arise. These can be personal, academic, political, commercial or financial – but have in common that they can corrupt science. Staff members are expected to act with integrity in every aspect of their work as researcher, reviewer, editor, and teacher. They should be impartial and independent from commissioning or interested parties, from ideological or political pressure groups, and from economic or financial interests. Conflicts of interest should be reported in publications and on staff members’ UvA profile page under Ancillary activities. They should not publish in journals or present at meetings, which are considered predatory because they accept papers/presentations without proper peer review or editorial control for their own profit.
Researchers should be honest in their communication not only to other scientists, but also to the general public, e.g., through the media. Researchers should not stretch the implications of their results beyond the scope of the study or make their scientific results appear too favorable.
It is highly recommended that researchers follow one of two approaches: preregistration or cross-validation. Both approaches ensure that transparency, accountability, and truth finding have the highest priority, and prevent questionable research practices. With preregistration, researchers stipulate their hypothesis and analysis plan in advance of data collection, tying their hands and letting the empirical chips fall where they may (Pierce, 1883). The theoretical advantage of preregistration is that it sharpens the distinction between two complementary but separate stages of scientific inquiry: the stage of generating hypotheses (i.e., exploratory research) and the stage of testing hypotheses (i.e., confirmatory research). By respecting this distinction, researchers inoculate themselves against the pervasive effects of hindsight bias and confirmation bias (e.g., Nuzzo, 2015). Another approach to the relationship of confirmatory and exploratory analysis is cross-validation. In such an analysis the data is split in two parts (this can be either at the level of participants or trials). The first part of the data is used for abduction/exploratory analysis, that is finding the best explanation for the observed data. Also, this data can also be used to optimize the pre-processing and processing steps often used in computational modelling and/or the analysis of EEG and fMRI data (by using k-fold crossvalidation). Next the developed hypothesis, with the selected processing pipe-line is tested, in a confirmatory way, on the second part of the data. Compared to pre-registration it provides transparency to the individual researcher about the status of the exploratory research by adding a confirmatory step, at the cost of having to acquire more data.
Errors in statistical analyses are common and are of a varied nature. For example, analyses may not fit the type of data, intended analyses may have not been carried out adequately, results may not have been reported adequately, or results may not have been interpreted adequately. Such errors may be less likely if four, instead of two, eyes are involved in analysis and reporting, the so-called co-pilot approach to data analysis. It is highly recommend that researchers follow a co-pilot approach at the UvA Psychology department. A co-pilot approach may decrease the likelihood of errors in data-analysis, thereby increasing the likelihood that conclusions are supported by the data, which in turn facilitates scientific progress.
If you have any doubts, questions or suspicions regarding scientific integrity, Joos Droogleever Fortuijn can be contacted within the Psychology Research Institute as special confidential advisor regarding scientific integrity.